Music has long been considered a uniquely human concept. In fact, most psychologists agree that music is a universal human instinct. Like any ability, however, there is great variation in people’s musical competence. For every brilliant pianist in the world, there are several people we refer to as “tone deaf”. 1It is not simply that people with tone deafness (or “amusia”) are unable to sing in tune, they are also unable to discriminate between tones or recognize familiar melodies. Such a “disorder” can occur after some sort of brain damage, but recently 10research has been undertaken in an attempt to discover the cause of congenital amusia (when people are born with the condition), which is not associated with any brain damage, hearing problems, or lack of exposure to music.
2According to the research of Dr. Isabelle Peretz of the University of Montreal, amusia is more complicated than the inability to distinguish pitches. An amusic (a person who has the condition of amusia) can distinguish between two pitches that are far apart, but 2cannot tell the difference between intervals smaller than a half step on the Western diatonic scale, while most people can easily distinguish differences smaller than that. 2When listening to melodies which have had a single note altered so that it is out of key with the rest of the melody, 2do not notice a problem. 2-11As would be expected, amusics perform significantly worse at singing and tapping a rhythm along with a melody than do non-amusics.
The most fascinating aspect of amusia is how specific to music it is. Because of music’s close ties to language, it might be expected that a musical impairment may be caused by a language impairment. 3Studies suggest, however, that language and music ability are independent of one another. People with brain damage in areas critical to language are often still able to sing, despite being unable to communicate through speech. Moreover, while amusics show deficiencies in their recognition of pitch differences in melodies, they show no tonal languages, such as Chinese, do not report having any difficulty discriminating between words that differ only in their intonation. 12The linguistic cues inherent in speech make discrimination of meaning much easier for amusics. 4Amusics are also successful most of the time at detecting the mood of a melody, can identify a speaker based on his or her voice and can discriminate and identify environmental sounds.
5Recent work has been focused on locating the part of the brain that is responsible for amusia. The temporal lobes of the brain, the location of the primary auditory cortex, 5have been considered. It has long been believed that the temporal lobes, especially the right temporal lobe, are most active when activity, so any musical disability should logically stem from here as well. Because it has been shown that there is no hearing deficit in amusia, 5researchers moved on to the temporal neocortex, which is where more sophisticated processing of musical cues was thought to take place. 5New studies, however, have suggested that the deficits in amusics are located outside the auditory cortex. Brain scans of amusics do not show any reaction at all to differences smaller than a half step. 13When changes in tones are large, their brains overreact, showing twice as much activity on the right side of the brain as a normal brain hearing the same thing. 5These differences do not occur in the auditory cortex, indicating again that the deficits of amusia lie mostly in hearing impairment, but in higher processing of melodies.
So what does this all mean? 6Looking only at the research of Peretz in the field of neuropsychology of music, it would appear that amusia is some sort of disorder. As a student of neurobiology, however, I am skeptical. Certainly the studies by Peretz that have found significant differences between the brains of so-called amusics and normal brains are legitimate. The more important question now becomes one of normality. Every trait from skin color to intelligence to mood exists on a continuum - there is a great idea of variation from one extreme to the other. Just because we recognize that basic musical ability is something that the vast majority of people have, this doesn’t mean that the lack of it is abnormal.
What makes an amusic worse off than a musical prodigy? 14Musical ability is culturally valued, and may have been a factor in survival at one point in human history, but it does not seem likely that it is being selected for on an evolutionary scale any longer. Darwin believed that music was adaptive as a way of finding a mate, but who needs to be able to sing to find a partner in an age when it is possible to express your emotions through a song on your iPod?
While the idea of amusia is interesting, it seems to be just one end of the continuum of innate musical ability. 8Comparing this “disorder” to learning disorders like a specific language impairment seems to be going too far. 7Before amusia can be declared a disability, further research must be done to determine whether lack of musical ability is actually detrimental in any way. 8-9If no disadvantages can be found of having amusia, then it is no more a disability9 than having poor fashion sense or bad handwriting.
Choose the correct letter, A, B, C or D. Write the correct letter in boxes 1-5 on your answer sheet
Do the following statements agree with the views of the writer in Reading Passage 3?
In boxes 6-9 on your answer sheet, write
YES if the statement agrees with the claims of the writer
NO if the statement contradicts the claims of the writer
NOT GIVEN if it is impossible to say what the writer thinks about this
Complete each sentence with the correct ending, A-H below. Write the correct letter, A-H in boxes 10-14 on your answer sheet.
A. an inability to hear when spoken language rises and falls. B. considered to be desirable. C. an inability to follow the beat of music. D. not a problem. E. not yet well understood. F. a result of injury to the mother. G. more marked that with other people. H. associated with intelligence. |